Paper-to-Podcast

Paper Summary

Title: Reducing Savings Gaps Through Pennies Versus Percent Framing


Source: Voya Financial


Authors: Stephen Shu et al.


Published Date: 2022-01-12

Podcast Transcript

Hello, and welcome to paper-to-podcast. Today, we'll be discussing a fascinating research paper I've only read about 35% of, but don't worry, I've got the gist of it. The paper is titled "Reducing Savings Gaps Through Pennies Versus Percent Framing" by Stephen Shu and colleagues. It was published on January 12, 2022, and it's all about how the way we frame savings can have a significant impact on how much people save.

So, the researchers tested a new strategy for presenting retirement savings choices to employees. They compared a pennies-based framing to the traditional percent-based framing. The results? Participants who were presented with the pennies framing planned to save an average of 29.5% of their salary, while those in the percent framing planned to save only 14.5%. That's quite the difference, huh?

In a real-world field study, participants who saw the pennies framing submitted higher savings rates (8.02%) than those who saw the percent framing (7.52%). The effects were even more significant for those with lower salaries (annual income less than $46,000) who increased their savings rates by approximately 115 basis points compared to a control savings rate of 6.88%. So, it seems that using a pennies-based framing could be a powerful tool to help reduce savings gaps and encourage better financial decisions, especially for those with lower incomes who may be less numerate or financially literate.

Now, the research involved two studies: a lab study and a field study. In the lab study, participants were presented with hypothetical retirement savings choices and asked to indicate their intended savings rates. The savings rates were framed either in terms of pennies per dollar earned or as a percentage of their salary. The field study was conducted in collaboration with a retirement services provider and involved non-auto enrollment plans from tax-exempt organizations, such as healthcare, education, and government sectors.

The most compelling aspects of the research are the use of a randomized controlled trial and the focus on addressing the retirement savings gap by applying behavioral science. By employing a floodlight-type analysis, the researchers were able to identify the groups most affected by the pennies framing.

Of course, there are some limitations to the research, such as the hypothetical nature of the choices in Study 1, the relatively small sample size and limited statistical power in Study 1, and the focus on non-auto-enrollment plans and tax-exempt organizations, which might limit the generalizability of the results. But hey, no research is perfect, right?

So, what can we do with these findings? Well, they could be used to develop more effective retirement savings plans for employees, especially those with lower incomes or less financial literacy. By using a pennies-based framing instead of percentages, employers could encourage higher savings rates and reduce inequalities between different income groups. This approach could also be applied to other financial decision-making contexts, such as personal savings and investment choices.

Policymakers and financial institutions could use these insights to design more inclusive financial products and services that cater to various levels of financial literacy. By making financial information more accessible and straightforward to understand, they can promote better financial decision-making and help close the gap between different income groups and levels of financial knowledge.

Moreover, the research could inspire further studies that explore the impact of different information architecture tools on various demographic groups and financial contexts. This could lead to a deeper understanding of how framing and presentation of financial information influence decision-making and ultimately contribute to the development of more effective financial education programs and policies.

That's all for today's episode. Remember, you can find this paper and more on the paper2podcast.com website. Thanks for joining us, and happy saving!

Supporting Analysis

Findings:
In this research, the authors tested a new strategy for presenting retirement savings choices to employees, comparing a pennies-based framing to the traditional percent-based framing. The results showed that the pennies-based framing led to higher intended savings rates in a hypothetical choice experiment. Participants in the pennies-free conditions planned to save an average of 29.5% of their salary, while those in the percent-free conditions planned to save only 14.5%. In a real-world field study, participants who saw the pennies framing submitted higher savings rates (8.02%) than those who saw the percent framing (7.52%). The effects were even more significant for those with lower salaries (annual income less than $46,000) who increased their savings rates by approximately 115 basis points compared to a control savings rate of 6.88%. These results suggest that using a pennies-based framing could be a powerful tool to help reduce savings gaps and encourage better financial decisions, especially for those with lower incomes who may be less numerate or financially literate.
Methods:
The research involved two studies: a lab study and a field study. In the lab study, participants were presented with hypothetical retirement savings choices and asked to indicate their intended savings rates. The savings rates were framed either in terms of pennies per dollar earned or as a percentage of their salary. The study also explored the role of subjective numeracy as a moderator in the relationship between the framing and intended savings rates. The field study was conducted in collaboration with a retirement services provider. It involved non-auto enrollment plans from tax-exempt organizations, such as healthcare, education, and government sectors. Participants were randomly assigned to either a pennies or percent treatment for making retirement savings selections. The researchers then examined the impact of the framing on actual savings rates submitted by the participants. The analysis included main effects, interaction effects, and floodlight analysis to determine how the framing affected people based on their subjective numeracy and income levels. Additionally, a structural equation model (SEM) analysis was performed as a robustness check for the lab study results, where perceptions of affordability and understandability were examined as potential mediators.
Strengths:
The most compelling aspects of the research are the use of a randomized controlled trial and the focus on addressing the retirement savings gap by applying behavioral science. The researchers utilized a 2x2 between-subject factorial design and carried out two studies: an online study with hypothetical choices (Study 1) and a field study (Study 2). Both studies aimed to understand the impact of framing savings choices in terms of pennies contributed per dollar earned rather than a percentage of salary. The researchers followed best practices by conducting rigorous experiments and exploring the effects of their intervention on different subpopulations, particularly those with lower income and numeracy levels. By employing a floodlight-type analysis, the researchers were able to identify the groups most affected by the pennies framing. Moreover, the study acknowledges the importance of information architecture as a tool for reducing inequality in behavioral outcomes and democratizing savings. Overall, the combination of a well-designed experiment with real-world implications, the focus on addressing a pressing societal issue, and the inclusion of both hypothetical and field studies, make this research compelling and contribute to the understanding of how behavioral interventions can improve financial decision-making.
Limitations:
Possible limitations of the research include the hypothetical nature of the choices in Study 1, which could lead to inflated savings rates compared to real-world decisions. Additionally, the relatively small sample size and limited statistical power in Study 1 could affect the detection of interactions between variables. In Study 2, the focus on non-auto-enrollment plans and tax-exempt organizations might limit the generalizability of the results to other types of retirement plans and organizations. Moreover, the use of income as a proxy for numeracy might not completely capture the effect of numeracy on savings decisions. Finally, the research relied on self-reported demographic information and subjective numeracy, which might be subject to biases and inaccuracies.
Applications:
The research findings could be used to develop more effective retirement savings plans for employees, especially those with lower incomes or less financial literacy. By using a pennies-based framing instead of percentages, employers could encourage higher savings rates and reduce inequalities between different income groups. This approach could also be applied to other financial decision-making contexts, such as personal savings and investment choices. Additionally, policymakers and financial institutions could use these insights to design more inclusive financial products and services that cater to various levels of financial literacy. By making financial information more accessible and straightforward to understand, they can promote better financial decision-making and help close the gap between different income groups and levels of financial knowledge. Moreover, the research could inspire further studies that explore the impact of different information architecture tools on various demographic groups and financial contexts. This could lead to a deeper understanding of how framing and presentation of financial information influence decision-making and ultimately contribute to the development of more effective financial education programs and policies.